- Get link
- X
- Other Apps
- Get link
- X
- Other Apps
High intelligence level in human makes them different from other creatures. It helps them to cope with complex situations of everyday life. The level of intellectual capacity differs from one individual to another and will determine how they will succeed in various activities they decide or are asked to undertake. It will also determine the performance in schools, the quality of decisions they make as well as the efficiency at work.
These benefits of
intelligence arouse interest of organisations such as schools or any other
business institutions in predicting the level of achievement of people in any
activities and where their services will be needed. It is for this reason a
highly controversial but very much beneficial intelligence quotient (IQ) test
was developed. A numerical value or score of intelligence that is obtained from
this test is based on the ratio between the people's mental age and
chronological age. This section will focus on different ways of measuring
intelligence.
9.1.1 THE DEVELOPMENT OF INTELLIGENCE TESTS
The idea of measuring intelligence began in 1904 in France, when the psychologist Alfred Binet was commissioned by the French government to find a method that would assist to differentiate between children who were ‘intellectually normal’ and those who were ‘inferior’ (slow learners). This was intended to put the latter into special schools where they would receive more individual attention. This would help the government to avoid the disruption they caused in the education of intellectually normal children.
Such thinking was a natural development from
Darwinism and Eugenic Movement that dates back to Sir Francis Galton in 1869,
the famous scientific polymath who promoted the idea that for the society to
prosper, the weakest should not have babies, as this would affect the genetic
stock of future generations. He and his followers were contemptuous of any
impact education might have on raising the achievement of the ‘least able’ .
Since,
according to Binet, intelligence could not be described by a single score, the
use of the intelligence quotient (IQ) as coined by Lewis, M. Terman in America
in 1916, as a definite statement of a child’s intellectual capacity would be a
serious mistake. In addition, Binet feared that IQ measurement would be used to
condemn a child to a permanent condition of stupidity, thereby negatively
affecting his/ her education and likelihood.
Despite such concerns,
psychometric tools use in learning institutions and work places gained a
significant momentum and in the various countries of the world. These tools
have been revised and modernised and typically include tests such as the
revised versions of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale or WAIS – R for 18
years old people and above; the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children or
WISC – III for children aged 6 to 17 ; the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale
of Intelligence or WPPSI for children 4 to 6 1/2 years old (Kasschau, 2003), and the British
Ability Scale or BAS in their updated forms but their roots and core constructs
remain unaltered.
9.1.2
The Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale
Binet’s intelligence test has been revised
many times since he developed it. In the United States of America, Binet's test was
refined by Lewis Terman of Stanford University into a test called
Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale. The Stanford-Binet, like the original test, groups test
items by age level. To stimulate and maintain the child’s interest, several
tasks are included, ranging from defining words to drawing pictures and
explaining events in daily life. Children are tested one at a time. Examiners
must carry out standardised instructions while putting the child at ease,
getting him to pay attention, and encouraging him to try as hard as he can.
Intelligence tests such as
these are used to predict how people will perform in situations or tasks that
seem to require intelligence like in school or on the job. Scores on
intelligence tests (IQ tests) indicate whether an individual has correctly
answered as many questions as the average person of his/her own age.
The IQ, or intelligence
quotient, defined as a measure of a person's intelligence as indicated by an
intelligence test is the ratio of a person's mental age (the
average age of those who also received the same score as that child) to their chronological age(actual)
multiplied by 100.
• For instance, if a Childs MA is 10 years and his CA is also 10 years, then his IQ is 100 (10/10× 100=100).
• IQs that
are over 100 (high IQ) show that the person is more intelligent than the
average person of his own age (the mental age is greater than the chronological
age). Example: if a Childs MA is 10 years and the CA is 8 years, then his IQ is
10/8 x100=125.
• IQs which
are less than 100 (lower IQ) indicate that the individual is less intelligent
than the average person of his own age (the mental age is lower than the
chronological age. For example, if a child's CA is 12 years and the MA is 9 years,
then his IQ is 9/12x100=75.
• Scores on
IQ tests indicate whether an individual has correctly answered as many
questions as the average person of his/her own age.
So an 8-year-old child who scored at the mental age of 8 would have an IQ of 100. Although the basic principles behind the calculation of IQ remain, scores are figured in a slightly different manner today. Researchers assign a score of 100 to the average performance at any given age. Then, IQ values are assigned to all the other test scores for this age group. If you have an IQ of 100, for example, this means that 50 per cent of the test takers who are your age performed worse than you.
In addition, test scores for several abilities are now reported
instead of one general score. Most learning institutions have done away with
this type of test; It was replaced by the Otis-Lennon Ability Test which seeks
to measure the cognitive abilities that are related to a student’s ability to
learn and succeed in school. It does this by assessing a student’s verbal and
nonverbal reasoning abilities.
Three frequently
used intelligence tests are the revised versions of the Wechsler-Adult
Intelligence Scale, or WAIS-R (Wechsler, 1981), for adults; the Wechsler
Intelligence Scale for Children, or WISC-III (Wechsler, 1981), for children 6
to 16 years old; and the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scales of
Intelligences, or WPPSI-R, for children 4 to 61⁄2 years old.
In addition to
yielding one overall score, the Wechsler tests yield percentile scores in
several areas, namely vocabulary, information, arithmetic, picture arrangement,
and so on as shown in the example of the Wechsler Tests below.
These ratings are used to compute separate IQ scores for verbal and performance abilities. This type of scoring provides a more detailed picture of the individual’s strengths and weaknesses than a single score does.
In most Wechsler
Intelligence Scales, the test items shown above are included although we did not
show all possible items. I am sure most of you have seen similar items in
Special Paper 2 of the End of Grade 7 National Examination in Zambia.
9.1.4 THE USES AND MEANING OF IQ SCORES
In general, the norms for intelligence tests are
established in such a way that most people score near 100. This means that about 95 percent of people score
between 70 and 130. The norms are illustrated in the following Gaussian Curve.
This normal curve displays intelligence as measured by IQ tests. Copyrighted to the owner |
As indicated on the curve, only a little more than 2 percent score at or above 130. These people are in at least the 97th percentile. Those who score below 70 have traditionally been classified as mentally handicapped. More specific categories include mildly handicapped, but educable (55–69); moderately handicapped, but trainable (40–54); severely handicapped (25–39); and profoundly handicapped (below 25).
IQ scores seem to be most
useful when related to school achievement; they are quite accurate in
predicting which people will do well in schools, colleges, and universities.
Critics of IQ testing do not question this predictive ability.
The question however remains,
whether such tests actually measure intelligence. Although most psychologists
agree that intelligence is the ability to acquire new ideas and new behaviour
and to adapt to new situations. Generally, IQ tests measure the ability to
solve certain types of problems. Yet they do not directly measure the ability
to pose those problems or to question the validity of problems posed by others (Hoffman, 1962). This is only part of the reason why IQ testing is so controversial.
CONTROVERSY OVER IQ TESTING
Much of the debate about IQ
testing centres around the following issues: do genetic differences or
environmental inequalities cause two people to receive different scores on
intelligence tests? The question of cultural bias in intelligence tests has
also been controversial.
1.
Nature vs. Nurture
A technique researchers use to
help determine whether genetics or environment affects scores on intelligence
tests is studying the results of testing of people with varying degrees of
genetic relationship. In regard to intelligence, researchers have found a high
degree of heritability or a measure of the degree to which a
characteristic is related to inherited genetic factors. They found that as
genetic relationship increases, say, from parent and child to identical twins,
the similarity of IQ also increases.
The best way to study the
effects of nature and nurture is to study identical twins who have been
separated at birth and raised in different environments. The study of more than
100 sets of twins who were raised apart from one another revealed that IQ is
affected by genetic factors. This finding was supported by the discovery of a
specific gene for human intelligence. According to some psychologists, 70
percent of IQ variance can be attributed to heredity, but others found the
hereditary estimate to be only 52 percent. This would mean that heredity
and environment almost contribute equally to the person’s intelligence.
In this Regard, studies on
environmental factors show that brothers and/or sisters raised in the same
environment are more likely to have similar IQs than siblings raised apart.
Environment, therefore, does impact IQs.
Some studies show that quality
preschool programs for example, help raise IQs initially, but the increase
begins to fade after some years. Participating children, however, are less
likely to be in special education classes, less likely to be held back, and
more likely to graduate from high school than are children without such
preschool experiences. Each year of school
missed may drop a person’s IQ as much as 5 points. The richness of
the home environment, the quality of food, and the number of brothers and
sisters in the family all affect IQ.
Both heredity and environment
have an impact on intelligence. Advances in behavioural genetics research
continue to refine results on the contributions that heredity and experience
have on IQ. It remains clear that these two factors are both contributing and
interact in their effects.
2.
Cultural Bias
A major criticism of
intelligence tests is that they have a cultural bias that is, the
wording used in questions may be more familiar to people of one social group
than to another group. For example, on one intelligence test the correct
response to the question, “What would you do if you were sent to buy a loaf of
bread and the grocer said he did not have any more?” was “try another store.” A
significant proportion of minority students, however, responded that they would
go home. When questioned about the answer, many explained that there was no
other store in their neighbourhood.
Psychologists admit that some
tests have been biased because they assess accumulated knowledge, which is
dependent on a child’s environment and opportunities in that environment. As a
consequence, efforts have been made to make the tests less biased. However, it
is unlikely that a test will ever be developed that will be completely free of
cultural bias. All tests are based on the assumptions of a particular culture.
An example of the culture bound test is shown below
This is a test developed by a psychologist, Adrian Dove in 1960 to demonstrate how one’s cultural background can affect their performance in an intelligence test.
Comments
Post a Comment